Book cover of Why Don’t Students Like School? by Daniel T. Willingham

Why Don’t Students Like School?

by Daniel T. Willingham

10 min readRating: 4.0 (5,378 ratings)
Genres
Buy full book on Amazon

Introduction

Have you ever wondered why so many students seem to dislike school? Daniel T. Willingham, a cognitive scientist and professor of psychology, tackles this question in his insightful book "Why Don't Students Like School?" Drawing on research in psychology and brain science, Willingham explores the cognitive processes involved in learning and memory, challenging common misconceptions about intelligence and education. This book offers valuable insights for parents, educators, and anyone interested in understanding how the human brain learns and how we can improve our educational systems.

The Brain's Reluctance to Think

One of the most surprising revelations in Willingham's book is that our brains actually don't like to think – at least not in the way we typically associate with "thinking." When we engage in complex cognitive tasks, such as solving difficult math problems or interpreting challenging texts, our brains consume a significant amount of energy. This energy-intensive process is something our brains naturally try to avoid.

This tendency can be traced back to our evolutionary past. In the days of our hunter-gatherer ancestors, conserving energy was crucial for survival. As a result, most of our brain power is devoted to processes that were more important for survival, like vision and movement. This is why we're exceptionally good at tasks like walking on uneven terrain or recognizing faces, but struggle with abstract thinking.

However, our brains have developed a clever workaround: pattern recognition. By spotting and recognizing patterns, we can quickly interpret situations without having to engage in energy-intensive thinking every time. This is how infants learn language – not through formal lessons, but by instinctively recognizing patterns in speech and connecting them to objects and situations.

The Two Types of Memory

Willingham explains that human memory can be divided into two main categories: working memory and long-term memory. Understanding these two types of memory is crucial for grasping how we learn and process information.

Working memory is like our brain's temporary workspace. It's where we hold and manipulate information that we're currently using or thinking about. For example, when you're trying to remember a phone number long enough to dial it, you're using your working memory. However, working memory has a very limited capacity – it can only hold about seven items at a time.

Long-term memory, on the other hand, is our brain's vast storage system. This is where we keep all the information we've learned and experiences we've had throughout our lives. Unlike working memory, long-term memory has an enormous capacity. The challenge lies in transferring information from working memory to long-term memory effectively.

The process of learning involves moving information from working memory to long-term memory. This transfer only happens if the brain considers the information important enough to store. Once information is in long-term memory, it can be recalled when needed, bringing it back into working memory for use.

Willingham uses a computer analogy to explain these two types of memory. Working memory is like a computer's RAM, which temporarily holds information needed for current tasks. Long-term memory is like a computer's hard drive, storing important data permanently. This analogy is particularly apt because early computer scientists actually modeled computer memory systems after the human brain!

The Importance of Context in Learning

One of the key insights Willingham offers is the critical role of context in learning. Our brains struggle to process new information in isolation; they prefer to have some prior knowledge or context to connect new information to. This is why learning a language that's similar to one you already know (like German for an English speaker) is generally easier than learning a completely unrelated language (like Japanese for an English speaker).

To illustrate this point, Willingham presents a paragraph of seemingly unrelated sentences about organizing items, color coordination, and not overdoing things. Without context, these sentences are confusing and require significant mental effort to understand. However, once you know they're instructions for using a washing machine, they make perfect sense. This demonstrates how crucial context is for comprehension and learning.

For educators, this insight has important implications. It suggests that teachers should:

  1. Ensure students have a solid grasp of basic principles in a subject before moving on to more complex topics. These foundational concepts provide the necessary context for understanding more advanced ideas.

  2. Use concrete examples to illustrate abstract concepts. This gives students a familiar framework to attach new, more abstract ideas to, making them easier to understand and remember.

The Role of Factual Knowledge in Critical Thinking

While many educators focus on developing students' critical thinking and analytical skills, Willingham argues that a strong foundation of factual knowledge is crucial for these higher-order thinking skills. He introduces the concept of "chunking" as a way to overcome the limitations of our working memory.

Chunking involves connecting bits of factual information in long-term memory, making it easier for working memory to handle complex reasoning tasks. For example, it's easier to remember the word "cognition" than to remember the nine individual letters that make it up. Similarly, when studying a complex topic like the Industrial Revolution, students need to first learn basic facts about technological innovations and historical context before they can effectively analyze and evaluate the economic changes of the period.

Willingham acknowledges that memorizing facts can seem tedious, but he emphasizes that there's no better way to store information in long-term memory. Moreover, as factual knowledge accumulates in long-term memory, the brain starts to make connections between different pieces of information, further enhancing understanding and critical thinking abilities.

Debunking Learning Styles

One of the most surprising and potentially controversial aspects of Willingham's book is his challenge to the widely accepted idea of learning styles. Many people believe they are visual, auditory, or tactile learners, preferring to receive information through a specific "channel." However, Willingham argues that this concept is largely a myth, unsupported by scientific research.

Numerous studies have shown that when students are given information in their supposed preferred style, there's no significant improvement in their academic performance. Willingham points out that visual, auditory, and tactile inputs are simply gateways for information to enter long-term memory. What truly matters is the meaning of the information, not how it's presented.

This doesn't mean that all students are identical in their abilities or preferences. Some may excel in math while others prefer literature. However, the idea that each student has a fixed, preferred learning style is not supported by evidence.

For educators, this insight suggests a shift in focus from delivery methods to content. Instead of spending excessive time on creating flashy presentations or trying to cater to different learning styles, teachers should concentrate on presenting information in a clear, meaningful way that helps students grasp the core concepts.

Intelligence: Nature vs. Nurture

Willingham tackles the age-old debate of nature versus nurture in relation to intelligence. For a long time, it was believed that children were born with fixed levels of intelligence and talent. However, recent research has challenged this assumption, showing that intelligence is the result of both genetics and environment.

While it's true that children are born with different levels of intelligence, Willingham emphasizes that these levels can be altered through sustained effort and environmental factors. In fact, environmental factors appear to be significantly more influential than genetic makeup in determining intelligence.

The author cites the Flynn effect as evidence of this. Since the 1930s, average IQ levels in many countries have increased significantly. For example, between 1952 and 1982, the average IQ of Dutch military draftees increased by 21 points. Such a dramatic change over a relatively short period can't be explained by genetic factors alone, as the human gene pool doesn't change that quickly.

This understanding has important implications for education. If students believe that intelligence is fixed and determined at birth, they may become demotivated when faced with challenges. On the other hand, if they understand that intelligence can be improved through effort and practice, they're more likely to persist in their studies and overcome difficulties.

The Importance of Teacher Development

While much of the book focuses on how students learn, Willingham also emphasizes the crucial role of teachers and their ongoing development. He argues that teaching is a complex skill that requires continuous improvement, just like any other profession.

Willingham points out that teachers' brains function in the same way as their students' brains. The same cognitive principles that lead to successful learning in students also apply to teachers improving their craft. However, many teachers plateau in their skills after the first few years of teaching.

To address this, Willingham advocates for a teaching culture that values feedback and continuous improvement. He suggests that teachers should regularly film themselves teaching and share these recordings with colleagues for constructive feedback. This practice can help teachers identify areas for improvement that they might miss on their own.

The author also emphasizes the importance of pedagogical content knowledge – not just knowing a subject, but knowing how to teach it effectively. This includes skills like explaining concepts clearly, managing interpersonal dynamics in the classroom, and resolving conflicts.

Practical Applications

Throughout the book, Willingham offers practical advice for educators and parents based on cognitive science principles. Some key recommendations include:

  1. Provide context: Always give students the necessary background information before introducing new concepts.

  2. Focus on content over delivery: Don't waste time trying to cater to different learning styles. Instead, concentrate on presenting information clearly and meaningfully.

  3. Encourage effort: Help students understand that intelligence and skills can be improved through practice and hard work.

  4. Build a strong foundation: Ensure students have a solid grasp of basic facts and principles before moving on to more complex topics.

  5. Use concrete examples: Illustrate abstract concepts with real-world examples that students can relate to.

  6. Promote teacher development: Encourage ongoing learning and improvement among educators through feedback and self-reflection.

Conclusion

"Why Don't Students Like School?" offers a fresh perspective on education, grounded in cognitive science and psychology. By understanding how our brains work – their limitations, preferences, and potential for growth – we can create more effective learning environments and teaching strategies.

Willingham challenges several common misconceptions about learning and intelligence, providing evidence-based insights that can transform our approach to education. He emphasizes the importance of factual knowledge, the role of context in learning, and the potential for intelligence to be developed through effort and practice.

For educators, the book offers valuable guidance on how to structure lessons, present information, and continually improve their teaching skills. For parents, it provides insights into how children learn and how to support their educational journey. And for students of all ages, it offers a new understanding of how our brains work and how we can become more effective learners.

Ultimately, Willingham's work suggests that by aligning our educational practices with the way our brains naturally function, we can not only improve learning outcomes but also make the process more engaging and enjoyable. Perhaps then, students might start to like school a little bit more.

As we move forward in the 21st century, facing new challenges and opportunities in education, the insights from "Why Don't Students Like School?" provide a valuable foundation for rethinking and improving our approach to teaching and learning. By bridging the gap between cognitive science and educational practice, we can create more effective, engaging, and enjoyable learning experiences for students of all ages.

Books like Why Don’t Students Like School?