Book cover of How Emotions Are Made by Lisa Feldman Barrett

How Emotions Are Made

by Lisa Feldman Barrett

10 min readRating: 4.1 (10,981 ratings)
Genres
Buy full book on Amazon

Introduction

Lisa Feldman Barrett's "How Emotions Are Made" challenges our conventional understanding of emotions. This groundbreaking book presents a new perspective on how our brains and cultures construct our emotional experiences. Barrett, a distinguished professor of psychology, argues against the classical view of emotions as hardwired, universal reactions. Instead, she proposes a theory of constructed emotion that sees our feelings as flexible, context-dependent creations of our brains.

The Classical View of Emotions

For centuries, the prevailing belief about emotions has been that they are innate, automatic responses triggered by specific events or stimuli. This classical view, supported by influential thinkers from Aristotle to Darwin and even modern psychologists, suggests that emotions are universal across cultures and hardwired into distinct regions of our brains.

According to this perspective, each emotion has a unique "fingerprint" – a specific set of physical and behavioral responses. For example, anger might always involve a raised heart rate and a scowling expression, while sadness might consistently lead to tears and a drooping posture.

This view also assumes that emotions are essentially reflexes, beyond our conscious control. It's the reason we often hear phrases like "you can't help how you feel" or see depictions of emotions as cartoon characters in our heads, like in the movie "Inside Out."

Challenging the Classical View

Barrett's research, however, reveals that scientific evidence doesn't support this classical understanding of emotions. Her work at the Interdisciplinary Affective Science Laboratory has shown that:

  1. There's no single, distinct way to express an emotion. Sadness, for instance, can manifest in various ways depending on the situation.

  2. Brain imaging studies have failed to identify specific regions dedicated to individual emotions. Instead, emotional experiences involve complex interactions across multiple brain areas.

  3. The same brain regions activated during emotional experiences are also active during non-emotional thoughts and perceptions.

These findings suggest that our emotions are far more nuanced and variable than previously thought. They aren't simple, automatic responses but complex constructions of our brains based on our past experiences and current context.

The Theory of Constructed Emotion

In place of the classical view, Barrett proposes the theory of constructed emotion. This theory suggests that emotions are created on the spot by our brains, using a combination of past experiences, bodily sensations, and cultural concepts.

Key aspects of this theory include:

  1. Emotions are not fixed or innate but are constructed in the moment.
  2. Our brains predict and interpret bodily sensations based on past experiences.
  3. Cultural concepts play a crucial role in shaping our emotional experiences.

This view of emotions is similar to how Darwin's theory of evolution challenged the concept of fixed biological essences. Just as species are populations of varying individuals adapting to their environments, our emotions are flexible constructions adapting to our current situations.

The Role of Interoception

A crucial component of Barrett's theory is the concept of interoception. This is the brain's system for monitoring and regulating the body's internal state, including things like heart rate, hormone levels, and energy use.

Interoception operates on two basic spectrums of affect:

  1. Pleasure to displeasure
  2. Agitation to calmness

These affects are the raw materials from which our brains construct more complex emotions. For example, the pleasant sensation of sunlight on your skin isn't an emotion in itself, but it might contribute to a feeling of happiness or contentment depending on the context and your past experiences.

The Body Budget

Barrett introduces the concept of the "body budget" to explain how our brains manage our body's resources. This budget includes things like glucose levels, heart rate, and hormone production. Our interoceptive system constantly monitors and adjusts this budget based on our current needs and anticipated future demands.

When our body budget becomes unbalanced – for instance, when we're low on energy or experiencing stress – our brain tries to make sense of this imbalance. This interpretation often results in what we experience as emotions. For example, if you're feeling worked up and your brain associates this with a threatening situation, you might experience fear.

Emotion Concepts and Cultural Construction

One of the most fascinating aspects of Barrett's theory is the role of culture in shaping our emotional experiences. She argues that emotion concepts – our ideas about what different emotions mean and how they should be expressed – are largely cultural constructions.

For example, the concept of "sadness" doesn't exist in Tahitian culture. Instead, they have a concept that roughly translates to "the kind of fatigue associated with the flu." This doesn't mean Tahitians don't experience what we might call sadness, but they conceptualize and express it differently.

Similarly, the association of smiling with happiness is a relatively recent cultural development. Ancient Greeks and Romans had no word for "smile," and it only became a significant gesture associated with happiness in the 18th century, partly due to increased access to dentistry.

These cultural variations show that our emotional experiences are shaped not just by our biology, but by the concepts and categories our cultures provide us with.

Learning Emotion Concepts

According to Barrett, we start learning emotion concepts from birth. Parents and caregivers teach babies to associate certain bodily sensations and situations with specific emotion labels. For instance, when a baby cries, a parent might ask, "Are you angry that it's nap time?" or "Are you sad that mommy is leaving for work?" These interactions help shape the child's understanding of what different emotions mean and when they're appropriate.

However, this learning process doesn't stop in childhood. Our brains retain the ability to learn new emotion concepts throughout our lives. This is why we can adopt new emotional vocabulary from other cultures or even create new concepts to describe our experiences.

For example, the German word "Schadenfreude" (pleasure derived from someone else's misfortune) has been adopted into English because it describes an emotional experience that didn't have a specific label in English before.

The Power of Emotional Granularity

Barrett emphasizes the importance of emotional granularity – the ability to distinguish between different emotional states with precision. People with high emotional granularity can differentiate between similar emotions (like frustration and irritation) and understand the nuances of their emotional experiences.

Developing greater emotional granularity can have several benefits:

  1. Better emotion regulation: When you can precisely identify your emotions, you're better equipped to manage them effectively.

  2. Improved communication: Being able to articulate your emotions clearly can lead to better relationships and social interactions.

  3. Enhanced well-being: Research suggests that people with higher emotional granularity tend to have better mental and physical health.

Implications for Emotional Intelligence

Barrett's theory has significant implications for our understanding of emotional intelligence. Rather than viewing emotional intelligence as the ability to recognize and manage fixed, universal emotions, it becomes about:

  1. Developing a rich vocabulary of emotion concepts
  2. Understanding how these concepts apply in different contexts
  3. Recognizing the cultural and personal factors that shape emotional experiences
  4. Being able to construct and regulate emotions flexibly based on the situation

This view suggests that emotional intelligence is a skill that can be developed through learning and practice, rather than an innate ability.

Practical Applications

Understanding emotions as constructions rather than fixed reactions opens up new possibilities for managing our emotional lives:

  1. Reframing emotions: If emotions aren't automatic responses, we have more power to reinterpret and reshape our emotional experiences.

  2. Expanding emotional vocabulary: Learning new emotion concepts can enrich our emotional experiences and give us more tools for understanding and expressing our feelings.

  3. Cultural awareness: Recognizing the cultural aspects of emotions can improve cross-cultural communication and empathy.

  4. Body budget management: Paying attention to our body's needs (sleep, nutrition, exercise) can help regulate our emotional states.

  5. Mindfulness: Being aware of our bodily sensations and the concepts we're using to interpret them can lead to more nuanced emotional experiences.

Challenges to Emotion Research and Technology

Barrett's theory poses significant challenges to traditional emotion research and technology. Many current approaches to studying and measuring emotions are based on the classical view that emotions have distinct, universal "fingerprints."

For example, facial recognition technology designed to detect emotions assumes that certain facial expressions always correspond to specific emotions. However, if emotions are constructed based on context and individual experiences, these technologies may be fundamentally flawed.

Barrett cites the example of SPOT (Screening Passengers by Observation Techniques), a program used by the Transportation Security Administration to identify potential terrorists based on facial and body movements. This program, which cost $900 million, was ultimately ineffective because it was based on faulty assumptions about how emotions are expressed.

This highlights the need for a more nuanced, context-sensitive approach to emotion research and technology.

Implications for Mental Health

The theory of constructed emotion also has important implications for our understanding and treatment of mental health issues:

  1. Depression and anxiety: Rather than viewing these as discrete, universal conditions, we might see them as culturally influenced constructions that can vary significantly between individuals and contexts.

  2. Emotion regulation: Treatment approaches might focus more on helping people construct more beneficial emotions rather than trying to control or suppress "negative" emotions.

  3. Cultural competence: Mental health professionals may need to be more aware of how cultural differences shape emotional experiences and expressions.

  4. Personalized treatment: Understanding emotions as constructions suggests that mental health treatments should be highly individualized, taking into account each person's unique experiences and cultural background.

The Future of Emotion Research

Barrett's work opens up exciting new avenues for emotion research:

  1. Interdisciplinary approaches: Studying emotions will require collaboration between neuroscientists, psychologists, anthropologists, and other disciplines.

  2. Focus on variation: Instead of looking for universal patterns, researchers might focus more on understanding the factors that contribute to emotional variation between individuals and cultures.

  3. Brain plasticity: More research is needed to understand how our brains learn and modify emotion concepts throughout our lives.

  4. Body-brain interactions: Further study of interoception and the body budget could provide insights into the physical basis of emotional experiences.

Conclusion

"How Emotions Are Made" presents a paradigm-shifting view of emotions that challenges many of our long-held beliefs. By proposing that emotions are constructed by our brains based on past experiences, bodily sensations, and cultural concepts, Barrett offers a more flexible and empowering understanding of our emotional lives.

This theory suggests that we have more control over our emotions than we might have thought. By expanding our emotional concepts, paying attention to our body budgets, and recognizing the role of culture in shaping our experiences, we can develop richer, more nuanced emotional lives.

While this new understanding of emotions poses challenges to existing research and technologies, it also opens up exciting possibilities for future study and application. From improving emotional intelligence to developing more effective mental health treatments, the theory of constructed emotion has far-reaching implications.

Ultimately, Barrett's work invites us to see emotions not as fixed, universal reactions, but as sophisticated constructions of our brains – tools that we can learn to use more effectively to navigate our complex social worlds. By embracing this view, we may gain a deeper understanding of ourselves and others, leading to more empathy, better communication, and improved well-being.

Books like How Emotions Are Made